Gao, Melanie
Melanie Gao
Personal Section
Hundreds of years ago, human beings worked with their hands and often walked from
place to place. Nowadays, muscle power is no longer in high demand and cars have replaced
walking; concurrently, modern society has created cheaper food markets, increasingly more
restaurants, and a multi-million dollar fast food industry. Recent technology has decreased the
need for physical activity while increasing the availability of food. Though these modern
advances are a blessing, they have also brought harmful consequences for the human population.
These conveniences cause individuals to consume more calories than they use; in general,
people’s energy consumption is greater than their energy expenditure. This condition has lead to
a recent phenomenon—the obesity epidemic.
First noted in the United States in 1980, the obesity epidemic has since increased twofold
in recent decades. In the 21st century, obesity has become one of the leading health problems in
the United States; over 34% of all adults age 20 years and over and 18% of all children age 6-11
years are obese (CDC Faststats, Obesity and Overweight). In fact, obesity is more prevalent in
the United States than in other Westernized country. With obesity comes a vast array of health
concerns including hypertension, glaucoma, cardiovascular disease, type 2 diabetes, high blood
pressure, certain cancers and heart disease. According to the National Institutes of Health,
obesity is one of the leading cause of preventable death in the US, second only to tobacco usage;
obesity-related conditions cause approximately 300,000 deaths a year.
As a sophomore in high school, I did not know much about the obesity epidemic and I
vaguely understood the concept of eating healthy and exercising regularly. When I first entered
the basement of the Neurological Institute of New York, I did not plan on applying to the Intel
1
Gao, Melanie
Competition nor had I decided to obesity research. Rather, I joined the Columbia University
Medical Center’s Program for Imaging and Cognitive Sciences (PICS) at the end of sophomore
year because a strange fascination with the brain. I found it fascinating how a three-pound
pudding of cells and circuits control the thoughts and habits that make up who we are. I was
determined to learn more about the brain’s function and awaited the many discoveries and
surprises ahead with enthusiasm. I was infected with the research bug.
In the lab, I worked on various projects with other researchers in the lab, ranging from an
autism study to a study on auditory processes. Independently, I developed an interest in hunger
and satiety. At that time, a mentor recommended David Kessler’s book, The End of Overeating,
which revealed shocking statistics about obesity in America and attempted to explain why
humans are so attracted to food. I wanted to understand why individuals found food so
mouthwateringly attractive, and how the brain normally controls food intake; using the resources
provided at the fMRI lab, I hope to better understand and prevent obesity and save lives. David
Kessler’s book ultimately became the basis for my Intel project investigating how individuals
respond to healthy food and junk food, and the particular salience of junk food.
Despite a two-hour commute via boat, subway and bus to the lab on the Upper West Side
of Manhattan, I have interned at the lab for almost two years. I had to learn basic computer
science and statistics to use the main software at the lab, Presentation, and to analyze data. My
experience at the lab has challenged me to think scientifically, tuning myself to ask the right
questions. There are just so many questions one can ask that cannot be pursued, for reasons of
practicality or time or simply because technology has not advanced far enough. At the same
time, there are so many that have been answered in every field of neurology, small steps trekked
on the uncharted territory of the brain by hundreds of scientists.
2
Gao, Melanie
Through my experience at the lab, I have acquired a better understanding of the difficulty
of doing science; research is not as picture perfect as it appears in science journals. I have
changed my original project idea countless times and repeated the same experiment multiple
times. As a scientist, one must have patience and one must truly enjoy doing science. To me,
science is a hobby; I enjoy spending time playing around with computer programming, reading
science articles, running studies on subjects, analyzing countless data sets and finding the
connections that lie underneath beneath the numbers. Research offers me the chance to satisfy an
almost insatiable curiosity and a thirst for knowledge and I plan to continue the exploration of
science I began in high school. I aspire to impact the scientific world, one data set at a time.
Research Section
Abstract:
In western cultures, food images can be found everywhere in the environment—from
large restaurant ads to fast food restaurants located at many intersections. Despite the high
prevalence of obesity and eating disorders, the factors and systems that regulate human eating
behavior remain poorly understood. Understanding the relationship between control mechanisms
and food stimuli and the factors that affect this relationship may provide a better understanding
of the factors underlying obesity, and may reveal better methods for individuals seeking to
regulate their eating behaviors and maintain a healthy weight. This pilot study developed a
unique Stroop paradigm using biologically relevant food images (the “Food Stroop”) to
investigate the relationship between junk (high-calorie) food, compared with healthy (low-
calorie) food, and control mechanisms. To do so, this study evaluated the response times and
accuracies of individuals identifying junk and healthy food stimuli in a Stroop task. The analysis
was done comparing congruency conditions between each trial and the trial preceding it; analysis
of the effect of BMI, gender, age and education level on response times was completed to
determine the translational relevance and effect of various demographics. This study revealed
3
Gao, Melanie
that not only did junk food cues elicit a significantly faster response from individuals, but it also
prevented potential conflict and is sustained longer in the memory trace compared to healthy
food cues. Junk food is more potent than healthy food is and induces faster processing. This
implicates separate neural processing mechanisms for junk and healthy food and suggests that a
unique neural network may exist to specifically process junk food images at a significantly faster
speed.
1. Introduction:
1.1.
Recent Prevalence of Obesity
The recent rise in obesity and food-related disorders is a major source of concern and
expense in the US and other westernized countries. In April 2009, Dr. David Kessler, former US
Surgeon General, published a best-selling book “The End of Overeating”, in which he
documents the rise in obesity and food-related health concerns, and identifies the consumption of
junk-food as the primary cause. He states that most individuals, despite differences in weight,
age and ethnicity, find it difficult to resist junk food. Importantly, Kessler concludes that the
primary defense against succumbing to the health perils of junk food must come from the
individual’s education about junk food’s power to overcome their innate cognitive control
mechanisms, which control individuals’ ability to choose a healthy diet. Unfortunately, very
little is known about cognitive mechanisms for controlling one’s appetite, specifically in the
presence of junk food. In addition, there is little understanding on the difference in responses
induced by healthy or “low-calorie” food cues and junk or “high-calorie” food cues.
Accordingly, this study specifically investigates, for the first time, a measure of individuals’
cognitive control relative to junk-food images. Using a Stroop task, this study was able to
identify quantitatively, the power of junk food images to overcome conflict situations and induce
faster cognitive processing.
1.2.
The Stroop Task
Since its introduction in 1935 (Stroop, 1935), the Stroop task has been used to monitor
cognitive conflict and derive models of cognitive control. In a typical Stroop task, the natural
4
Gao, Melanie
processing of task-irrelevant stimuli (distracters) often
interferes with an
individual’s
performance on an identification task. In the classic Stroop task, participants are instructed to
identify the ink color of a color word in congruent and incongruent conditions. For example, in
the congruent condition, the stimuli consists of color words printed with the same color ink (i.e.,
the word RED printed in red ink), while in the incongruent condition, color words are printed in
a different ink color (i.e., the word RED printed in green ink). By comparing the response times
in these two conditions, an interference effect can be observed. The conflict in incongruent
conditions causes impaired cognitive performance; the “Stroop interference effect” is derived
from observations that participants are generally slower to identify ink colors in the incongruent
condition than in the congruent condition. These participants are distracted by the word because
reading is a more automatic process than color identification and thus, more difficult to inhibit
(Macleod & Macdonald, 2000). Interestingly, the interference, or “conflict”, produced by
incongruent task-irrelevant information in the incongruent condition is reduced if the
incongruent trial is preceded by another incongruent trial (incongruent-incongruent), compared
with when an incongruent trial is preceded by a congruent trial (congruent-incongruent) (Egner
and Hirsch 2005a; Notebaert et al. 2006). The superior performance on the second consecutive
task reveals an improved conflict resolution and suggests that the brain can rapidly adjust
processing mechanisms when exposed to consecutive conflict situations. This improved conflict
resolution mechanism helps overcome the conflict and induces better performance on the second
incongruent task. The “Conflict adaptation effect” describes the improved conflict resolution
following the presentation of two consecutive incongruent stimuli.
1.3 Food as a Biologically Relevant Stimuli
Food consumption is one of the most important human behaviors. Many factors influence
a person’s attraction to certain foods; certain physical characteristics of food, such as taste, smell
and appearance affect an individual’s attraction to food. Furthermore, through learning and
experience, the visual characteristics of food can act as reinforcers, capable of inducing eating-
related behavior (Lappalainen and Sjoden, 1992; Killgore et al., 2003). Food is salient because,
5